Developing a New Measure of the Useful Field of View

People are faced with numerous distractions every day in their lives, and multi-tasking has become the new way of getting things done. However, this may not be a good thing - complex environments require much of our attention and we only have limited resources for processing information. By attempting to focus on more than one task at a time, performance across all tasks often decreases. This is especially apparent when studying the useful field of view (UFOV). The UFOV contains everything that a person can see in their visual field in one eye fixation, without moving their eyes. However, the extent of the UFOV is not fixed, but can change - training can expand the UFOV (Ball et al., 1988), while adding a cognitive load can diminish it.

Introducing a cognitive load, usually in the form of a demanding secondary task that requires working memory resources, can make people overlook events or objects within their UFOV, or even glance directly at something but not remember seeing it. This can happen in a variety of situations; a conspicuous example is a person driving while distracted by a cell-phone, not noticing a bicyclist on the side of the road. After first finding a reliable way to measure the UFOV, training could then be implemented to teach people to function under the stress of a cognitive load and protect their UFOV against deterioration. This would be especially useful for drivers or pilots.

This research project, funded by the Office of Naval Research (ONR), is focused on developing a new measure of moment-to-moment changes to the UFOV due to stress, fatigue, and cognitive load, which can be used in dynamic viewing environments such as simulators and virtual reality.  A key idea is to use gaze-contingent displays (GCDs), which use eyetracking to change a computerized display depending on where the viewer is looking at any given moment.

Blur Detection

blur-detection-task 

n-back

The initial studies used detection of gaze-contingent image blur in natural scenes as our dependent measure of the UFOV, allowing participants to freely gaze at images and indicate when they had seen blur appear. To test the breadth of the UFOV, blur would appear at one of four different retinal eccentricities (0°, 3°, 6°, and 9°), starting at the center of vision and moving farther into the visual periphery (picture shown above). Cognitive load was then manipulated by adding the N-back task (picture shown above) as a secondary task, with various levels of difficulty.  The N-back task was tested in both an audio and visual format. It was hypothesized that blur detection sensitivity would decrease as the N-back task became more difficult, either causing general interference or tunnel vision.

This video illustrates our gaze-contingent blur detection experiment. The green dot moving around the image represents the subject's eye movements and was not present during the actual experiment. Gaze-contingent blur appeared every seventh fixation, with the exception of catch-trials.

This video illustrates our gaze-contingent blur detection experiment with an auditory N-back task. An N-back letter was presented every 2 seconds. Please make sure that the volume on your computer is turned up in order to hear the N-back letters.

This video illustrates our gaze-contingent blur detection experiment with a visual N-back task. An N-back letter appeared at the point of fixation every 3 seconds and remained in place if a subject moved their eyes.

The results of these studies showed a significant main effect of eccentricity on blur detection - that is, at the center of vision, image blur that was very subtle could be detected, but in order to detect blur in the visual periphery, it needed to be stronger.  However, adding the N-back task had no effect on blur detection sensitivity, indicating that blur detection was unaffected by cognitive load. This was a surprising result, and could suggest that blur detection is actually perceived pre-attentively. However, we are currently conducting follow-up research to test whether this lack of effect of the N-back task on blur detection sensitivity was due to the N-back task having been too easy for our subjects.

A presentation for this project was recently presented at the Vision Sciences Society Annual Meeting in Naples, FL.

Gabor Orientation Discrimination

gabor-discrimination

One of our recent studies, conducted at the Beckman Institute using a driving simulator, employed a different dependent variable to measure the field of view, moving from the blur detection task to a Gabor orientation discrimination task (four black and white, slightly off-vertical Gabor patches are shown in the picture above. Click here for the video that shows the Gabor discrimination task in the driving simulator).  We made this change because, while blur may be detected pre-attentively, Gabor orientation discrimination is known to be affected by attention and also because discrimination tasks tend to show stronger effects of attention than detection tasks.  This study also used GCDs to ensure that the Gabor patches appeared at varying retinal eccentricities, depending on where the participant was looking. In this study, participants carried out a driving task in a driving simulator while carrying out two other tasks: 1) when the Gabor patches appeared, they decided whether the Gabors were vertical or not, and 2) at the same time, participants also performed an auditory N-back task.

The results of this study showed that performance on the Gabor orientation discrimination task was affected by adding cognitive load with the N-back task, indicating that the Gabor task is indeed influenced by attention. Therefore, we will use the Gabor orientation discrimination task in our on-going research on creating a new dynamic measure of transient changes to the UFOV.

This video illustrates a gaze-contingent Gabor orientation discrimination task in a natural scene, which is being used in current studies. The green dot moving around the image represents the subject's eye movements and was not present during the actual experiment.

Collaborators for this project include Dr. Arthur Kramer, University of Illlinois at Urbana-Champaign; Dr. Aaron Johnson, Concordia University, Montreal; and Dr. Mark Neider, University of Central Florida.

 

Related Publications

Ringer, R. V., Johnson, A. P., Gaspar, J. G., Neider, M. B., Crowell, J., Kramer, A. F., & Loschky, L. C. (2014). Creating a new dynamic measure of the useful field of view using gaze-contingent displays. Proceedings of the Symposium on Eye Tracking Research and Applications (pp. 59-66): ACM.

Related Conference Presentations

Ringer, R.V., Dean, K.E., Larson, A.M., Coy, A., Walton, T., Sprat, J., Clark-Hargreaves, L., Johnson, A., Neider, M., Kramer, A., &Loschky, L.C. (2013, May). Blur detection in natural scenes is not affected by cognitive load. Poster session presented at the Vision Sciences Society Annual Meeting, Naples, FL.

Loschky, L. C., Ringer, R. V., Larson, A. M., Hughes, G. A., Dean, K., Weiser, J., Flippo, L., Johnson, A. P., Neider, M. B. & Kramer, A. F. (2012, June). Developing a Gaze-Contingent Measure of the Useful Field of View in Dynamic Scenes.  Invited Talk presented at the Fifth International Conference on Cognitive Science, Kaliningrad (former Königsberg), Russia.

Loschky, L. C., Ringer, R. V., Larson, A. M., Hughes, G. A., Dean, K., Weiser, J., Flippo, L., Johnson, A. P., Neider, M. B., & Kramer, A. F. (2012, May).  Developing a New Measure of the Useful Field of View for Use in Dynamic Real-World Scene Viewing.  Poster presented at the 12th annual meeting of the Vision Sciences Society, Naples, FL.